Resumen
Antecedentes: El crecimiento y diversificación de las relaciones entre humanos y otras especies animales ha suscitado el interés por la consideración o trato moral hacia dichas especies. Desde la cognición moral, se vienen estudiando tanto las tendencias de razonamiento, como la percepción de capacidades cognitivas que los humanos ven como importantes para considerar a un animal no humano digno de trato moral. Objetivos: El presente artículo realiza una reflexión respecto del denominado círculo moral como espacio psicológico, del cual se incluyen o excluyen agentes desde la percepción de capacidades y creencias asociadas a su estatus moral. Así, se exploran conceptos como el antropocentrismo o especismo, que han sido asociados a la naturaleza de las relaciones entre humanos y otras especies, para profundizar en el conocimiento que se tiene sobre los mismos en el desarrollo infantil. Resultados: La indagación realizada desde la cognición moral en el desarrollo lleva a conocer los antecedentes y cambios en la consideración moral de animales no humanos, además de permitir reflexionar en torno diversas formas de abordar y ampliar el estudio de la inclusión de animales en el círculo moral. Conclusión: Por ello, la parte final del artículo propone acudir a conceptos como la teleología y el desentendimiento moral para seguir profundizando en las bases cognitivas del trato moral a otras especies desde temprana edad, dadas las implicaciones en la educación y la situación ambiental actual.
Referencias
Amiot, C. E., & Bastian, B. (2015). Toward a psychology of human-animal relations. Psychological Bulletin, 141(1), 6–47. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0038147
Amiot, C. E., & Bastian, B. (2017). Solidarity with animals: Assessing a relevant dimension of social identification with animals. PLoS ONE, 12. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168184
Amiot, C. E., Sukhanova, K., Greenaway K. H., & Bastian B. (2017) Does Human– Animal Similarity Lower the Need to Affirm Humans’ Superiority Relative to Animals? A Social Psychological Viewpoint, Anthrozoös, 30:3, 499-516, https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2017.1335117
Ankomah, W. S. (2018). Animal studies: Let’s talk about animal welfare and liberation issues in childhood. Society and Animals, 28(3), 311–326. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685306-12341565
Auger, B., & Amiot, C. E. (2019). Testing the roles of intergroup anxiety and inclusion of animals in the self as mechanisms that underpin the “pets as ambassadors” effect. Anthrozoos, 32, 5–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2019.1550277
Awad, E., Dsouza, S., Kim, R., Schulz, J., Henrich, J., Shariff, A., Rahwan, I. (2018). The Moral Machine experiment. Nature, 563(7729), 59–64. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0637-6
Bandura, A. (2001). SOCIAL COGNITIVE T HEORY: An Agentic Perspective.
Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 1–26. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1 n
Bandura, A. (2004). Selective exercise of moral agency. In H. J. Thorkildsen, T.A & Walberg (Ed.), Nurturing Morality (1st ed., pp. 37–57). Boston: Kluwer Academic.
Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of personality and social psychology, 71(2), 364. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022- 3514.71.2.364
Banerjee, K., & Bloom, P. (2015). "Everything happens for a reason": children's beliefs about purpose in life events. Child development, 86(2), 503–518. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12312
Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2010). Excluded from humanity: The dehumanizing effects of social ostracism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 107–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.022
Bastian, B., & Loughnan, S. (2017). Resolving the Meat-Paradox: A Motivational Account of Morally Troublesome Behavior and Its Maintenance. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(3), 278–299. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868316647562
Bastian, B., Loughnan, S., Haslam, N., & Radke, H. R. M. (2012). Don’t mind meat? the denial of mind to animals used for human consumption. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(2), 247–256. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211424291
Berenguer, J. (2010). The Effect of Empathy in Environmental Moral Reasoning.
Environment and Behavior, 42, 110–134. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916508325892
Bilewicz, M., Imhoff, R., & Drogosz, M. (2011). The humanity of what we eat: Conceptions of human uniqueness among vegetarians and omnivores. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 201-209. DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.766
Bloom, P. (2010). How do morals change? Nature, 464, 490. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/464490a
Buttelmann, D., & Böhm, R. (2014). The Ontogeny of the Motivation That Underlies In-Group Bias. Psychological Science, 25(4), 921-
927. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0956797613516802
Bone, J. (2013). The animal as fourth educator: A literature review of animals and young children in pedagogical relationships. Australian Journal of Early Childhood, 38(2), 57–64. https://doi.org/10.1177/183693911303800208
Bratanova, B., Loughnan, S., & Bastian, B. (2011). The effect of categorization as food on the perceived moral standing of animals. Appetite, 57(1), 193–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.04.020
Busch, J. T., Watson-Jones, R. E., & Legare, C. H. (2017). The coexistence of natural and supernatural explanations within and across domains and development. The British journal of developmental psychology, 35(1), 4–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12164
Caviola, L., & Capraro, V. (2020). Liking but Devaluing Animals: Emotional and Deliberative Paths to Speciesism. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(8), 1080–1088. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1948550619893959
Caviola, L., Everett, J. A. C., & Faber, N. S. (2019). The moral standing of animals: Towards a psychology of speciesism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(6), 1011–1029. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000182
Christensen, J. , Flexas, A. , Calabrese, M. , Gut, N. , & Gomila, A. (2014). Moral judgment reloaded: a moral dilemma validation study. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00607
Clement, G. (2013). Animals and Moral Agency: The Recent Debate and Its Implications. Journal of Animal Ethics, 3(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.5406/janimalethics.3.1.0001
Climaterra. (Junio de 2020). El fin de la carne: crecen el vegetarianismo y el veganismo. Climaterra. Recuperado de https://www.climaterra.org/post/el-fin- de-la-carne-crecen-el-vegetarianismo-y-el-veganismo
Costello, K., & Hodson, G. (2014a). Lay beliefs about the causes of and solutions to dehumanization and prejudice: Do nonexperts recognize the role of human- animal relations? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44(4), 278–288. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12221
Costello, K., & Hodson, G. (2014b). Explaining dehumanization among children: the interspecies model of prejudice. The British journal of social psychology, 53(1), 175–197. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12016
Crimston, D., Bain, P. G., Hornsey, M. J., & Bastian, B. (2016). Moral expansiveness: Examining variability in the extension of the moral world.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(4), 636–653. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000086
Cushman, F. (2015). Deconstructing intent to reconstruct morality. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 97–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.06.003
Cushman, F., Sheketoff, R., Wharton, S., & Carey, S. (2013). The development of intent-based moral judgment. Cognition, 127(1), 6–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.008
Dhont, K., & Hodson, G. (2014). Why do right-wing adherents engage in more animal exploitation and meat consumption? Personality and Individual Differences, 64, 12-17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.02.002
Dhont, K., Hodson, G., & Leite, A. C. (2016). Common Ideological Roots of Speciesism and Generalized Ethnic Prejudice: The Social Dominance Human–Animal Relations Model (SD-HARM). European Journal of Personality, 30(6), 507–522. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2069
DiYanni, C., & Kelemen, D. (2005). Time to get a new mountain? The role of function in children’s conceptions of natural kinds. Cognition, 97(3), 327–335. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.10.002
Dovidio, J. F., Gaterner, S. L. (2001). Intergroup Bias. In G. Fiske, S., Gilbert, D, Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of Social Psychology (5th ed., pp. 1084–1121). Wiley
Duhn, I. (2012). Places for pedagogies: Pedagogies for places. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 13(2), 99–106. https://doi.org/10.2304%2Fciec.2012.13.2.99
El Tiempo (26 de enero de 2018). Cada vez son más los colombianos que tienen mascota. El Tiempo. Recuperado de https://www.eltiempo.com/colombia/cada-vez-son-mas-los-colombianos-que- tienen-mascota-175608
Fonseca, M. J., Franco, N. H., Brosseron, F., Tavares, F., Olsson, I. A. S., & Borlido-Santos, J. (2011). Children’s attitudes towards animals: Evidence from the RODENTIA project. Journal of Biological Education, 45(3), 121–128. https://doi.org/10.1080/00219266.2011.576259
Geerdts, M. S., Van de Walle, G. A., & LoBue, V. (2015). Daily animal exposure and children’s biological concepts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 132–146. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.10.001
Gini, G., Pozzoli, T., & Hymel, S. (2014). Moral disengagement among children and youth: A meta-analytic review of links to aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 40(1), 56–68. http://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21502
Guevara Benavides L.M. (18 de febrero 2019). Seis de cada 10 hogares del país tienen mascota según Brandstrat. La República. Recuperado de https://www.larepublica.co/consumo/seis-de-cada-10-hogares-del-pais-tienen- mascota-segun-brandstrat-2829114
Harris, P. L. (2012). Trusting what you’re told: How children learn from others.
Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.
Harris, P. L., & Koenig, M. A. (2006). Trust in testimony: How children learn about science and religion. Child Development, 77, 505–524. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00886.x
Heiphetz, L., & Young, L. (2014). A social cognitive developmental perspective on moral judgment. Behaviour, 151(2–3), 315–335. http://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-00003131
Hester, N., & Gray, K. (2020). The Moral Psychology of Raceless, Genderless Strangers. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(2), 216–230. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619885840
Hussar, K.M. and Harris, P.L. (2010), Children Who Choose Not to Eat Meat: A Study of Early Moral Decision‐making. Social Development, 19: 627-
641. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00547.x
Hussar, K. M., & Horvath, J. C. (2011). Do children play fair with mother nature?
Understanding children’s judgments of environmentally harmful actions. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 31(4), 309–313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2011.05.001
Janoff-Bulman, R., & Carnes, N. C. (2013). Surveying the moral landscape: moral motives and group-based moralities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17(3), 219–36. http://doi.org/10.1177/1088868313480274
Kahn, P. H. (1997a). Children’s moral and ecological reasoning about the Prince William Sound oil spill. Developmental Psychology, Vol. 33, pp. 1091–1096. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.33.6.1091
Kahn, P. H. (1997b). Developmental Psychology and the Biophilia Hypothesis: Children’s Affiliation with Nature. Developmental Review, 17(1), 1–61. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1996.0430
Kahn P. H., Jr (1997). Bayous and jungle rivers: cross-cultural perspectives on children's environmental moral reasoning. New directions for child development, (76), 23–36. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.23219977604
Kahn, P. H., & Lourenço, O. (2002). Water, Air, Fire, and Earth: A Developmental Study in Portugal of Environmental Moral Reasoning. Environment and Behavior, 34(4), 405–430. https://doi.org/10.1177/00116502034004001
Kahn, P. H., Severson, R. L., & Ruckert, J. H. (2009). The human relation with nature and technological nature. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(1), 37–42. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01602.x
Kansky, R. and Knight, A. T. 2014. Key factors driving attitudes towards large mammals in conflict with humans. Biological Conservation 179, 93–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2014.09.008
Kelemen, D. (1999). The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children.
Cognition, 70(3), 241–272. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00010-4 Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children “intuitive theists”? Reasoning about purpose and
design in nature. Psychological Science, 15(5), 295–301. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00672.x
Kortenkamp, K. V., & Moore, C. F. (2001). Ecocentrism and anthropocentrism: Moral reasoning about ecological commons dilemmas. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21, 261-272. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1006/jevp.2001.0205
Legare, C. H., & Lombrozo, T. (2014). Selective effects of explanation on learning during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 198– 212. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.03.001
Lombrozo, T., & Carey, S. (2006). Functional explanation and the function of explanation. Cognition, 99(2), 167–204. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.12.009
Loughnan, S., Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2014). The Psychology of Eating Animals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(2), 104–108. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414525781
Loughnan, S., Haslam, N., & Bastian, B. (2010). The role of meat consumption in the denial of moral status and mind to meat animals. Appetite, 55(1), 156–159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.05.043
Malle, B. F. (2011). Attribution Theories: How People Make Sense of Behavior. En
D. Chaade (Ed.), Theories in Social Psychology. (1st ed., pp. 72–95). Wiley- Blackwell
Malle, B. F., Guglielmo, S., & Monroe, A. E. (2014). A Theory of Blame.
Psychological Inquiry, 25(2), 147–186. http://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2014.877340
Melo Cortés T. (17 de junio de 2019). Hay más de 600 millones de vegetarianos en el mundo. RCN Radio. Recuperado de https://www.rcnradio.com/estilo-de- vida/hay-mas-de-600-millones-de-vegetarianos-en-el-mundo
Melson, G. F. (2013). Children’s ideas about the moral standing and social welfare of non-human species. Journal of Sociology and Social Welfare, 40(4), 81– 106.
Melson, G. F., & Fine, A. H. (2010). Animals in the Lives of Children. In A. H. Fine (Ed.), Handbook on Animal-Assisted Therapy (Third Edit, Vol. 2008, pp. 223– 245). https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-381453-1.10012-1
Melson, G. F., Kahn, P. H., Beck, A., Friedman, B., Roberts, T., Garrett, E., & Gill,
B. T. (2009). Children’s behavior toward and understanding of robotic and living dogs. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 92–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.10.011
Miralles, A., Raymond, M., & Lecointre, G. (2019). Empathy and compassion toward other species decrease with evolutionary divergence time. Scientific reports, 9(1), 19555. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-56006-9
Mercier, H. (2011). What good is moral reasoning? Mind & Society, 10(2), 131–148 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-011-0085-6
Moore, C. (2015). Moral disengagement. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 199– 204. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.07.018
Moreno-Romero, C.O. (2020). Desentendimiento moral y atribución de culpa: Encuentros y desencuentros en el estudio de la cognición moral. Revista Colombiana de Psicología, 29, 125-141. https://doi.org/10.15446/rcp.v29n1.76536
Myers, O.E., Jr. (2007). The significance of children and animals: Social development and our connection to other species (rev. ed.). West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University Press.
Neldner K, Crimston C, Wilks M, Redshaw J, Nielsen M. (2018) The developmental origins of moral concern: An examination of moral boundary decision making
throughout childhood. PLOS ONE 13(5) https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197819
Nucci, L., & Turiel, E. (2009). Capturing the complexity of moral development and education. Mind, brain, and education, 3(3), 151-
159. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-228X.2009.01065.x
Obermann, M.-L. (2013). Temporal Aspects of Moral Disengagement in School Bullying: Crystallization or Escalation? Journal of School Violence, 12(January 2015), 193–210. http://doi.org/10.1080/15388220.2013.766133
Olthof, T., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., Lalay-Cederburg, C., Reijntjes, A., & Hagenaar, J. (2008). The assignment of moral status: Age-related differences in the use of three mental capacity criteria. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26(2), 233–247. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151007X216036
Overton, W. (2013). A new paradigm for developmental science: Relationism and relational- developmental systems. Applied Developmental Science, 17, 94- 107. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2013.778717
Piaget, J. (1965/1932). The moral judgment of the child. New York: Free Press.
Piazza, J., & Loughnan, S. (2016). When Meat Gets Personal, Animals’ Minds Matter Less: Motivated Use of Intelligence Information in Judgments of Moral Standing. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(8), 867–874. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616660159
Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature: Why violence has declined.
London, England: Penguin.
Pozzoli, T., Gini, G., & Vieno, A. (2012). Individual and Class Moral Disengagement in Bullying Among Elementary School Children. Aggressive Behavior, 38(5), 378–388. http://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21442
Rhodes, M., Leslie, S. J., & Tworek, C. M. (2012). Cultural transmission of social essentialism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(34), 13526–13531. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1208951109
Rosch, E. (1999). Principles of categorization. In E. Margolis & S. Laurence (Eds.),
Concepts: Core readings (pp. 189-206). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Rothgerber, H. (2013). Real men don’t eat (vegetable) quiche: Masculinity and the
justification of meat consumption. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 14(4), 363–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030379
Rozin, P. (2007). Food and eating. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), The handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 391–416). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Ruby, M. B., & Heine, S. J. (2011). Meat, morals, and masculinity. Appetite, 56(2), 447–450. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.018
Ruby,M. B. (2012). Vegetarianism. A blossoming field of study. Appetite, 58, 141– 150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.09.019
Ruckert, J. H. (2016). Justice For All ? Children ’ s Moral Reasoning about the Welfare and Rights of Endangered Species Justice For All ? Children ’ s Moral Reasoning about the Welfare and Rights of Endangered Species. 7936(June). https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2015.1093297
Sanbonmatsu, J. (2014). The animal of bad faith: Speciesism as an existential project. Critical Animal Studies: Thinking the Unthinkable, (June), 29–45. Recuperado de https://www.researchgate.net/profile/John_Sanbonmatsu/publication/2794491 14_The_Animal_of_Bad_Faith_Speciesism_as_an_Existential_Project/links/5 592f2a908ae5af2b0eb65a7.pdf
Schein, C. (2020). The Importance of Context in Moral Judgments. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(2), 207–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620904083
Severson, R. L., & Kahn, P. H. (2010). In the orchard: Farm worker children’s moral and environmental reasoning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31(3), 249–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2010.02.003
Severson, R. L., & Lemm, K. M. (2016). Kids see human too: Adapting an individual differences measure of anthropomorphism for a child sample. Journal of Cognition and Development, 17, 122–141. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2014.989445
Sevillano, V., & Fiske, S. T. (2016). Warmth and competence in animals. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 46, 276–293. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12361
Sevillano, V., & Fiske, S. T. (2019). Stereotypes, emotions, and behaviors associated with animals: A causal test of the stereotype content model and BIAS map. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 22(6), 879–900. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430219851560
Sloman, S. A., & Lagnado, D. (2015). Causality in Thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 223–247. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015135
Smetana, J. G., Jambon, M., Conry-Murray, C., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2012).
Reciprocal Associations between Young Children’s Developing Moral Judgments and Theory of Mind. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1144– 1155. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0025891
Sommer, K., Nielsen, M., Draheim, M., Redshaw, J., Vanman, E. J., & Wilks, M. (2019). Children’s perceptions of the moral worth of live agents, robots, and inanimate objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 187, 104656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.06.009
Šorytė, D., & Pakalniškienė, V. (2019). Why it is important to protect the environment: reasons given by children. International Research in Geographical and Environmental Education, 28(3), 228–241. https://doi.org/10.1080/10382046.2019.1582771
Timm, S. C. (2016). Moral Intuition or Moral Disengagement? Cognitive Science Weighs in on the Animal Ethics Debate. Neuroethics, 9(3), 225–234. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-016-9271-x
Topolski, J. R., Weaver, N., Martin, Z., & McCoy J. (2013) Choosing between
the Emotional Dog and the Rational Pal: A Moral Dilemma with a Tail, Anthrozoös: A multidisciplinary journal of the interactions of people and animals, 26:2, 253- 263 http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rfan20
Townsend, D., Taylor, L. K., Merrilees, C. E., et al. (2020). Youth in Northern Ireland: Linking Violence Exposure, Emotional Insecurity, and the Political
Macrosystem. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 85(4). https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12423
Uskul, A. K., & Oishi, S. (2020). Editorial overview: What is socio-ecological psychology? Current Opinion in Psychology, 32, 181–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.01.001
Waytz, A., Cacioppo, J., & Epley, N. (2010). Who Sees Human?: The Stability and Importance of Individual Differences in Anthropomorphism. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(3), 219 -
232. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610369336
Waytz, A., Gray, K., Epley, N., & Wegner, D. M. (2010). Causes and consequences of mind perception. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14, 383-388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.006
Wilks, M., Caviola, L., Kahane, G., & Bloom, P. (2021). Children Prioritize Humans Over Animals Less Than Adults Do. Psychological Science, 32(1), 27–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620960398
Zhang, Z., Grocke, P., & Tomasello, M. (2019). The influence of intention and
outcome on young children’s reciprocal sharing. Journal of experimental child psychology, 187, 104645. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.05.012
Esta obra está bajo una licencia internacional Creative Commons Atribución-NoComercial-CompartirIgual 4.0.
Derechos de autor 2022 Tesis Psicológica